A Site With A Bias For Health,Wealth And Relationship.
How extreme fear shapes what we remember.
Get link
Facebook
X
Pinterest
Email
Other Apps
(Getty Images)
Many of us will experience a significant trauma in our lives, says
Lesley Evans Ogden. Yet could there be ways to avoid reliving the
memories?
It was no ordinary honeymoon. After boarding their flight in Canada
on the evening of 23 August 2001, newlyweds Margaret McKinnon and her
husband were heading for Lisbon, Portugal. As Air Transat flight 236
soared over the mid-Atlantic, McKinnon went to the lavatory. Nothing
inside it was working. “It seemed odd,” she says, but she didn't think a
lot of it.
The fear system has evolved to keep us alive — Karim Nader, McGill University
Returning
to her seat, the crew served breakfast, but then announced that they
would be making an emergency landing. She remembers thinking it seemed
early to be arriving in Lisbon. “I didn't really understand at the time
what that meant,” she says. She soon found out. Crew instructed
passengers to put on their life jackets. The lights flickered, then
extinguished. The cabin depressurised. Oxygen masks deployed.
The
plane’s systems had shut down after a catastrophic leakage of fuel.
“They were shouting that we would be ditching into the ocean,” McKinnon
recalls.
After a half hour of preparing for the worst, McKinnon
recalls somebody yelling that they’d made it to land. It was the Azores,
an isolated archipelago some 850 miles (1,360km) off the Portuguese
coast. The pilots had established contact with Lajes, a joint
military-civilian air base. Following a harrowing 360 degree spin and
several sharp turns to reduce altitude, the crew shouted “brace, brace,
brace” as the officers brought the plane to a bumpy landing. Fires
licked across the plane’s wheels.
(Getty Images)
The stunned passengers and crew descended escape
slides and ran across a field to a safe distance, towards American
soldiers with guns. Two serious and 16 minor injuries were sustained
during evacuation down the chutes, but all 293 passengers and 13 crew
survived.
But for many the flight didn’t end there. For some –
including McKinnon – the terrifying experience replayed vividly as
intrusive memories and nightmares in the months that followed.
The
experience inspired McKinnon, now a clinical psychologist, to study
what trauma does to the brain – how it changes what we remember and why
some people experience Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). In recent
years, she and a number of other researchers have been trying to
understand what makes fearful experiences seem to become imprinted so
deeply in our brains. And if they can understand why trauma has such a
profound and lasting effect on us, perhaps they can find ways to help
people cope better with the aftermath. Fearful imprint
The
link between fear and memory has intrigued researchers and clinicians
for decades. Yet the data is conflicting. “Some studies have found that
during the recollection of traumatic events, recollection is enhanced.
It's very vivid, people recall many details, and people don't seem to
have difficulty remembering,” says McKinnon. Other studies have found
that recollection of traumatic events can be very impoverished and
fragmented, with “a detail here, a detail there, that don't really fit
together”, she explains.
(Getty Images)
Few studies have looked at memory during the
experience of trauma itself, especially for a single, shared event. So
McKinnon decided to delve into the memories of her fellow passengers on
Air Transat flight 236.
“We wanted to take this opportunity to
look at a very ‘controlled’ circumstance, so to speak,” says McKinnon,
suppressing an awkward laugh at using “controlled” to describe AT 236’s
emergency descent. McKinnon and collaborators examined the memories for
15 passengers on the flight, comparing their recollection of three
events: the flight itself, an emotionally neutral event from the same
year, and their experience during 9/11, the following month. Six of
those interviewed exhibited PTSD symptoms.
The researchers guided
subjects’ memories by saying “tell me everything you can remember about
the event”, and then further aided retrieval with cues like “what were
you thinking, what were you feeling, and what was the lighting like in
the cabin?” These detailed recollections were compared with the known
sequence of events, and a control group who had suffered less traumatic
memories.
What they found was that all passengers, regardless of
whether they went on to develop PTSD, had vivid and enhanced
recollections of the incident, supporting the idea that fear changes how
the brain stores memories. In those that subsequently developed PTSD,
“they showed a lot of recollection of extraneous details, not only of
the traumatic event, but also for the events of September 11, as well as
from the neutral memory from the same time period”, says McKinnon. It
suggests that these individuals have difficulty editing what they
recollect, or fading the contents of memory.
McKinnon admits that
her study represents a small sample, and is cautious about generalising
the results, but they are nevertheless intriguing. “Understandably,
people were reluctant to participate,” says McKinnon. “And we are very,
very grateful to the people who did participate, because it can be
difficult to talk about these kinds of things.”
(SPL)
So if traumatic memories are more vivid, what’s
going on inside our heads when they are ‘made’? There are multiple
memory systems in the brain. We have physical memories, like how we
learn to ride a bike. We have auditory memories for singing songs. And
we have more specific “declarative” memory systems heavily involving the
hippocampus. The hippocampus stores memory for things like where we
parked our car, and that two plus two is four.
But fear activates a different system: our body’s emergency control centre – the amygdala.
The amygdala, a pair of almond-shaped structures on the left and right
of the brain’s medial temporal lobe, is particularly involved in
emotional memories like fear, but also in pleasurable memories
associated with food, sex, or recreational drug use. When a memory is
particularly striking and unexpected, it activates this emotional memory
system.
That may be partly why there are a plethora of anecdotes
about how sensory cues, out of context, can take you right back to
emotional memories – perhaps you associate the scent of a certain
perfume or cologne with your first kiss because the memory is higher
fidelity.
(Getty Images)
With a fearful experience, our survival systems
kick into action, and we can achieve what’s known as a single trial
memory. “If you escape from a lion once, or you see someone else being
eaten by a lion, you know to be afraid of that lion,” explains Kerry
Ressler, a professor of psychiatry and behavioural sciences at Emory
University in Atlanta, Georgia. That's very different from something
like studying facts in a book, and activities that aren’t emotionally
arousing. “That may make sense evolutionarily, because we want to
prioritise things that are really important,” explains Ressler.
When
we feel fear, a burst of adrenaline activates a cascade that is thought
to enhance memory storage of the immediately preceding events. “The
fear system has evolved to keep us alive,” says Karim Nader, a
psychology professor at McGill University in Montreal, Canada. Flashbulb memories
Fearful
experiences may not always lay down strong memories, however. Elizabeth
Phelps, a professor of psychology and neural science at New York
University, was interested in people’s so-called “flashbulb memories” of
9/11. These are not individuals with PTSD, “but everyday people who had
experienced 9/11, which was most of us”, says Phelps. And curiously,
she found that, vivid as these memories feel, they were not as strong as
people believed – they could be changed.
(Getty Images)
Phelps’ lab at NYU was near the thick of the
disaster. In a large-scale detailed survey conducted first within a few
weeks of 9/11, then one year, two years, and 10 years later, they found
that “people were very confident that the details of their memory were
correct”. Not the detail in terms of the fact that it happened, but
where they were, who they were with, how they first heard about it, and
what they did afterwards. However, personal recollections of the
contextual details often actually changed with time.
This
suggests, says Phelps, that flashbulb memories differ from memories of
more neutral events not because the details of the memory are preserved
any better, but because we think they are. “With highly traumatic events
we think we have this incredibly accurate memory,” she says. The truth
is, many of the details we think are accurate are not. “Emotion focuses
your attention on a few details, at the expense of a lot of others,”
explains Phelps. Total recall
So, could this mean that traumatic memories can be manipulated, even removed?
Based on an emerging understanding of the storage and retrieval of
memory, we have windows of opportunity for altering the closure of
fearful memories in the brain. The time window for dampening the initial
locking-in of memories, explains Nader, is on the order of 6 hours. So
pharmacological approaches to lessen the strength with which a
distressing memory is locked into the brain need to be administered
within that short window. Indeed, there is emerging evidence from rodent
and human studies that drugs called beta-blockers can achieve a
reduction in later PTSD symptoms if administered quickly. “The Israeli
military now use that,” says Nader. But new research is suggesting that
even beyond the short window during which memories are saved to the
internal hard drive of our brain, they can be retrieved, updated, and
toned down.
(Thinkstock)
Experimenting with rats, Nader reminded animals
of a conditioned fearful memory with a musical tone (previously linked
to an electric shock), and subsequently administered a beta-blocker.
Even after the beta-blocker was eliminated from the blood, the fearful
freezing response to the memory was gone. Exploring the same approach in
a small sample of humans, Nader and colleagues determined that despite
an average of 11 years between trauma and experimental intervention,
“even after the beta-blocker was eliminated from the blood, their trauma
was down to non-PTSD levels”. It’s still early days for this method,
and studies of its efficacy are ongoing.
Still, by using what we
know about reconsolidation as a potential therapeutic tool, it seems
that a subtle form of memory reprogramming is within reach. “We’re not
changing your knowledge of what happened. We’re just changing its
association with these fight or flight stress responses that we get”,
says Phelps. “So the notion that we’re going to be able to erase your
knowledge of what happened – that’s science fiction at this point.” Departure points
As for McKinnon, she admits that despite her vividly traumatic experience, there are many details she can’t remember.
“We
were over the top of the island and then the plane veered back over the
ocean, which was incredibly scary, because then we thought ‘that was
it’,” says McKinnon, recalling the moment that AT 236 approached the
Lajes landing strip. She remembers seeing the tops of houses, and
worrying about crashing there and killing other people. But asked if the
plane was in darkness during its terrifying descent, McKinnon says, “I
have to be honest with you – I entirely don't remember.” Nor can she
remember whether or not she was sitting beside the window.
When it
comes to terrifying memories, perhaps our brains are selective in what
details are stored. The more we learn about how and why, the closer
we’ll get to lessening the impact of trauma.
That fated honeymoon
flight was an eventful way to begin a marriage. “It was quite a start,
yes,” McKinnon laughs. “Unexpected.” It proved to be a life-changing
year in more than one way: making her aware of the need for research,
but also for better care and treatment for PTSD, she says. “And that was
the direction I wanted to take my career in.” McKinnon’s experience
that day was the start of an unforeseen new personal and scientific
journey. As time goes by, the memory may be harder to remember, but the
legacy is hard to forget.
Civil service instigates investigation into leaked memo from Foreign Office about supposed comments made in February Scotland’s first minister, Nicola Sturgeon, with David Cameron in her office at the Scottish parliament earlier this year. Photograph: WPA Pool/Getty A civil service inquiry into a leaked memo which claimed that Nicola Sturgeon privately wanted to see David Cameron remain in power after the general election has been instigated following calls from the First Minister. Ms Sturgeon described the allegation as “100% untrue” and accused Whitehall of “dirty tricks”.
Health workers take passengers' temperatures infrared digital laser thermometers at the Felix Houphouet Boigny international airport in Abidjan on Aug. 13, 2014. Ivory Coast on Monday banned air travellers from Guinea, Liberia and Sierra Leone, the three countries worst-hit by the Ebola outbreak, and ordered its flagship carrier Air Cote d'Ivoire to cease flights to and from them. Reuters/Luc Gnago Guinea, one of the worst-hit West African nations in the ongoing Ebola outbreak, announced a state of emergency Wednesday. The World Health Organization, or WHO, also said that four new people died in Guinea between Aug. 10 and Aug. 11. The total death count in Guinea from the latest epidemic was estimated at 377 by WHO while the number of cases reported had risen to ...
Macbook, March 2015 (image: Apple PR) Taking a look back at another week of news from Cupertino, this week’s Apple Loop looks at Apple’s rush to remove iOS 8.2, three new iPhone 7 handsets, the iPad Pro feature set, emotions behind the Apple Watch, the wristband accessory guidelines, Applebot debuts, streaming music services and Beats, and the Apple Watch’s tattoo issue.